This week’s article summary is 3
Reasons Most Teachers Still Believe the Learning Styles Myth, and it’s a
follow-up to last week’s summary which focused more generally on the persistent
popularity of many learning myths.
This week’s article deals specifically with the myth of
learning styles.
The article’s author, Daniel Willingham, in his book Why
Student Don't Like School, my favorite education book, explains why
teaching to learning styles is a myth.
Yes, most of us have a preferred sense for receiving
information—visual, auditory, kinesthetic. However, the ultimate key to
learning is not how information is received but how it is stored (and then
retrieved) from long-term memory.
And, as last week’s article outlined, there are
research-supported techniques that help us store content in memory more
successfully than others. As an example, frequently revisiting content over a
long period time is a much more effective technique than highlighting in a
textbook as it makes learning much more active and over time stimulates and
myelinates brain synapses. (I can’t wait to brag to my parents that I used the
word ‘myelinates’ in a sentence.)
Willingham’s article below surmises on the reasons why so
many of us continue to believe in this myth.
Yes, good pedagogy ensures different senses are stimulated
but also provides research-supported ways for students to store and remember
content knowledge.
Joe
-----
I concluded that many teachers believe learning-styles
theory is accurate in about 2003. It was perhaps the second or third time I had
given a public talk to teachers. I mentioned it in passing as an example of a
theory that sounds plausible but is wrong, and I felt an immediate change in
the air. Several people said “Wait, what? Can you please back up a slide?”
Since then I’ve written a couple of articles about
learning styles, created a video on the
subject, and put an FAQ on
my website. Recently I was on
NPR’s Science Friday radio program to talk about learning styles and
other neuromyths.
I put energy into dispelling the learning styles myth
because I thought that audience of educators was representative—that is, that
most teachers think the theory is right.
In recent surveys learning styles theory was endorsed by 93%
of the public and 76% of educators.
Why is acceptance of the idea so high? No one really knows,
but here’s my tripartite guess.
First, I think by this point it’s achieved the status of one
of those ideas that “They” have figured out. People believe it for the same
reason I believe atomic theory. I’ve never seen the scientific papers
supporting it, but everyone believes the theory and my teachers taught it to
me, so why would I doubt that it’s right?
Second, I think learning styles theory is widely accepted
because the idea is so appealing. It would be so nice if it were true.
It predicts that a struggling student would find much of school work easier if
we made a relatively minor change to lesson plans—make sure the auditory
learners are listening, the visual learners are watching, and so on.
Third, something quite close to the theory is not only
right, it’s obvious. The style distinctions (visual versus
auditory; verbal versus visual) often correspond to real differences in
ability. Some people are better with words, some with space, and so on. The
(incorrect) twist that learning styles theory adds is to suggest that everyone
can reach the same cognitive goal via these different abilities; that if I’m
good with space but bad with words (or better, if I prefer space
to words), you can rearrange a verbal task so that it plays to my spatial
strength.
That’s where the idea goes wrong. First, the reason we make
the distinction between types of tasks is that they are separable in the brain
and mind; we think verbal and visual are fundamentally different, not fungible.
Second, while there are tasks that can be tackled in more than one way, these
tasks are usually much easier when done in one way or another. For example, if
I give you a list of concrete nouns, one at a time, and ask you to remember
them, you could do this task verbally (by repeating the word to yourself,
thinking of meaning, etc.) or visually (by creating a visual mental image).
Even for people who are not very good at imagery, the latter method is a better
method of doing the task. People’s alleged learning styles don’t count for
anything in accounting for task performance, but the effect of a strategy on a
task are huge.
A final note: I frequently hear from teachers that they
learned about the theory in teacher education classes. I've looked at all of
the well-known educational psychology textbooks, and none of them present the
idea as correct. But neither do they debunk it. Debunking these ideas in ed
psych textbooks ought to help.
No comments:
Post a Comment